APA Research Paper (Shaw)
Apes and Language:
A Review of the Literature
Over the past 30 years, researchers have demonstrated that
the great apes (chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans) resemble
humans in language abilities more than had been thought possible.
Just how far that resemblance extends, however, has been a
matter of some controversy. Researchers agree that the apes have
acquired fairly large vocabularies in American Sign Language and
in artificial languages, but they have drawn quite different conclusions
in addressing the following questions:
1. How spontaneously have apes used language?
2. How creatively have apes used language?
3. Can apes create sentences?
4. What are the implications of the ape language studies?
This review of the literature on apes and language focuses on
these four questions.
How Spontaneously Have
Apes Used Language?
In an influential article, Terrace, Petitto, Sanders, and Bever
(1979) argued that the apes in language experiments were not
using language spontaneously but were merely imitating their
trainers, responding to conscious or unconscious cues. Terrace and
his colleagues at Columbia University had trained a chimpanzee,
Nim, in American Sign Language, so their skepticism about the
apes’ abilities received much attention. In fact, funding for ape
language research was sharply reduced following publication of
their 1979 article “Can an Ape Create a Sentence?”
In retrospect, the conclusions of Terrace et al. seem to have
been premature. Although some early ape language studies had
not been rigorously controlled to eliminate cuing, even as early
as the 1970s R. A. Gardner and B. T. Gardner were conducting
double-blind experiments that prevented any possibility of cuing
(Fouts, 1997, p. 99). Since 1979, researchers have diligently
guarded against cuing.
Perhaps the best evidence that apes are not merely
responding to cues is that they have signed to one another
spontaneously, without trainers present. Like many of the apes
studied, gorillas Koko and Michael have been observed signing
to one another (Patterson & Linden, 1981). At Central Washington
University the baby chimpanzee Loulis, placed in the
care of the signing chimpanzee Washoe, mastered nearly fifty
signs in American Sign Language without help from humans.
“Interestingly,” wrote researcher Fouts (1997), “Loulis did not
pick up any of the seven signs that we [humans] used around
him. He learned only from Washoe and [another chimp] Ally”
(p. 244).
The extent to which chimpanzees spontaneously use language
may depend on their training. Terrace trained Nim using the
behaviorist technique of operant conditioning, so it is not surprising
that many of Nim’s signs were cued. Many other researchers
have used a conversational approach that parallels the process by
which human children acquire language. In an experimental study,
O’Sullivan and Yeager (1989) contrasted the two techniques, using
Terrace’s Nim as their subject. They found that Nim’s use of
language was significantly more spontaneous under conversational
conditions.
How Creatively Have
Apes Used Language?
There is considerable evidence that apes have invented
creative names. One of the earliest and most controversial examples
involved the Gardners’ chimpanzee Washoe. Washoe, who knew
signs for “water” and “bird,” once signed “water bird” when in the
presence of a swan. Terrace et al. (1979) suggested that there was
“no basis for concluding that Washoe was characterizing the swan
as a ‘bird that inhabits water.’” Washoe may simply have been
“identifying correctly a body of water and a bird, in that order”
(p. 895).
Other examples are not so easily explained away. The
bonobo Kanzi has requested particular films by combining symbols
on a computer in a creative way. For instance, to ask for
Quest forFire,
a film about early primates discovering fire, Kanzi began touse symbols for “campfire” and “TV” (Eckholm, 1985). The gorilla
Koko, who learned American Sign Language, has a long list of
creative names to her credit: “elephant baby” to describe a Pinocchio
doll, “finger bracelet” to describe a ring, “bottle match” to
describe a cigarette lighter, and so on (Patterson & Linden, 1981,
p. 146). If Terrace’s analysis of the “water bird” example is applied
to the examples just mentioned, it does not hold. Surely
Koko did not first see an elephant and then a baby before signing
“elephant baby”--or a bottle and a match before signing “bottle
match.”
Can Apes Create Sentences?
The early ape language studies offered little proof that apes
could combine symbols into grammatically ordered sentences. Apes
strung together various signs, but the sequences were often random
and repetitious. Nim’s series of sixteen signs is a case in point:
“give orange me give eat orange me eat orange give me eat orange
give me you” (Terrace et al., 1979, p. 895).
More recent studies with bonobos at the Language Research
Center in Atlanta have broken new ground. Kanzi, a bonobo trained
by Savage-Rumbaugh, seems to understand simple grammatical
rules about word order. For instance, Kanzi learned that in twoword
utterances action precedes object, an ordering also used by
human children at the two-word stage. In a major article reporting
on their research, Greenfield and Savage-Rumbaugh (1990) wrote
that Kanzi rarely “repeated himself or formed combinations that
were semantically unrelated” (p. 556).
More important, Kanzi began on his own to create certain
patterns that may not exist in English but can be found among
deaf children and in other human languages. For example, Kanzi
used his own rules when combining action symbols. Symbols
that involved an invitation to play, such as “chase,” would
appear first; symbols that indicated what was to be done during
play (“hide”) would appear second. Kanzi also created his own
rules when combining gestures and symbols. He would use
the symbol first and then gesture, a practice often followed by
young deaf children (Greenfield & Savage-Rumbaugh, 1990,
p. 560).
In a later study, Kanzi’s abilities to understand spoken
language were shown to be similar to those of a 2-1/2-year-old
human, Alia. Rumbaugh (1995) reported that “Kanzi’s comprehension
of over 600 novel sentences of request was very comparable
to Alia’s; both complied with the requests without assistance on
approximately 70% of the sentences” (p. 722). A recent monograph
provided examples of the kinds of sentences both Kanzi and
Alia were able to understand:
For example, the word ball occurred in 76 different sentences,
including such different requests as “Put the leaves in your
ball,” “Show me the ball that’s on TV,” “Vacuum your ball,” and
“Go do ball slapping with Liz.” Overall, 144 different content
words, many of which were presented in ways that required syntactic
parsing for a proper response (such as “Knife your ball”
vs. “Put the knife in the hat”), were utilized in the study.
(Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 2000, pp. 101-102)
The researchers concluded that neither Kanzi nor Alia could have
demonstrated understanding of such requests without comprehending
syntactical relationships among the words in a sentence.
What Are the Implications of the
Ape Language Studies?
Kanzi’s linguistic abilities are so impressive that they may
help us understand how humans came to acquire language. Pointing
out that 99% of our genetic material is held in common with
the chimpanzees, Greenfield and Savage-Rumbaugh (1990) have
suggested that something of the “evolutionary root of human language”
can be found in the “linguistic abilities of the great apes”
(p. 540). Noting that apes’ brains are similar to those of our
human ancestors, Leakey and Lewin (1992) argued that in ape
brains “the cognitive foundations on which human language could
be built are already present” (p. 244).
The suggestion that there is a continuity in the linguistic
abilities of apes and humans has created much controversy.
Linguist Noam Chomsky has strongly asserted that language is
a unique human characteristic (Booth, 1990). Terrace has continued
to be skeptical of the claims made for the apes, as have
Petitto and Bever, coauthors of the 1979 article that caused such
skepticism earlier (Gibbons, 1991).
Recently, neurobiologists have made discoveries that may
cause even the skeptics to take notice. Ongoing studies at the
Yerkes Primate Research Center have revealed remarkable similarities
in the brains of chimpanzees and humans. Through brain scans
of live chimpanzees, researchers have found that, as with humans,
“the language-controlling PT [planum temporale] is larger on the
left side of the chimps’ brain than on the right. But it is not lateralized
in monkeys, which are less closely related to humans than
apes are” (Begley, 1998, p. 57).
Although the ape language studies continue to generate
controversy, researchers have shown over the past 30 years that
the gap between the linguistic abilities of apes and humans is far
less dramatic than was once believed.
References
Begley, S. (1998, January 19). Aping language. Newsweek, 131,
56-58.
Booth, W. (1990, October 29). Monkeying with language: Is chimp
using words or merely aping handlers? The Washington Post, p.
A3.
Eckholm, E. (1985, June 25). Kanzi the chimp: A life in science.
The New York Times, pp. C1, C3.
Fouts, R. (1997).
Next of kin: What chimpanzees have taught meabout who we are.
New York: William Morrow.Gibbons, A. (1991). Déjà vu all over again: Chimp-language wars.
Science, 251, 1561-1562.
Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). Grammatical
combination in Pan paniscus: Processes of learning and invention
in the evolution and development of language.
In S. T. Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.),
“Language” and intelligencein monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives
(pp. 540-578). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Leakey, R., & Lewin, R. (1992).
Origins reconsidered: In search ofwhat makes us human.
New York: Doubleday.O’Sullivan, C., & Yeager, C. P. (1989). Communicative context and
linguistic competence: The effect of social setting on
a chimpanzee’s conversational skill. In R. A. Gardner, B. T.
Gardner, & T. E. Van Cantfort (Eds.),
Teaching sign language tochimpanzees
(pp. 269-279). Albany: SUNY Press.Patterson, F., & Linden, E. (1981). The education of Koko.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Rumbaugh, D. (1995). Primate language and cognition:
Common ground. Social Research, 62, 711-730.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J. S., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke,
K. E., Williams, S. L., Rumbaugh, D. M., et al. (2000).
Languagecomprehension in ape and child: Monograph.
Atlanta, GA:Language Research Center. Retrieved January 6, 2000, from the
Language Research Center Web site: http://
www.gsu.edu/~wwwlrc/monograph.html
Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979).
Can an ape create a sentence? Science, 206,
891-902.
0 comments:
Post a Comment